According to estimates by the Robert Koch Institute (RKI), up to 2,000 hearing-impaired children are born in Germany each year. For some of them, a cochlear implant can offer relief. Until now, it was not clear which processes take place in the affected children when they start to learn language later than their contemporaries with normal hearing—and why they differ in their success to reach a normal level of language. A current study at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig and University Medical Centre Dresden found that deaf children with a cochlear implant learn words even faster than those with normal hearing, the Max Planck Institute announced on its website. This finding can help refine the search for the reasons behind their varying success in language acquisition.

For many years, scientists attempted to find a replacement for a damaged or dysplastic inner ear. Cochlear implants receive a sound, convert it into electrical stimuli, and send these impulses directly to the auditory nerve, thereby giving hearing-impaired children the chance to connect to the world of sounds and noises.

It has so far been assumed that these children reach the language level of children with normal hearing, but much later. Previous studies have shown that from the moment of having the device implanted, children need longer to attain the important steps of learning their mother tongue—for instance, being able to distinguish the rhythm of their mother tongue from that of another language. This could imply that developmental milestones necessary to start school are also delayed, although they reach all the other developmental stages needed.

A current study at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig and the University Medical Centre Dresden has now revealed something different.

“We observed that when deaf children get their implants, they learn words faster than those with normal hearing. Consequently, they build up certain word pools faster”, says Niki Vavatzanidis, first author of the underlying study and scientist at MPI CBS and the University Medical Centre Dresden. Normally, children need 14 months to reliably recognize that known objects are named incorrectly. Children with an artificial cochlea were already able to do so after 12 months.

The reason for this finding could be that children with cochlear implants are older when they are first exposed to spoken language. Those with normal hearing learn aspects of language, such as the rhythm and melody of their mother tongue, from birth and even in the womb. In deaf children, this only starts at the time of their cochlear replacement, at the age of around 1-4 years. By this time, certain brain structures necessary for language acquisition are already well-developed.

“It is not just the memory, but also the broader knowledge about their surroundings that is more formed. They already know about objects in their environment and have accumulated non-linguistic semantic categories,” said Vavatzanidis. For example, they already know that objects such as cups or meals could be hot and that heat could be something harmful without knowing the word “hot.”

The scientist analysed the brain activities of the little ones with cochlear implant using electroencephalography (EEG). © MPI CBS

The scientist analyzed the brain activities of the participants with cochlear implants using electroencephalography (EEG). © MPI CBS

The neuroscientists examined these relations with the help of 32 children with cochlear implants in both ears. They carried out a test 12, 18, and 24 months after implantation that gauged the participants’ ability to recognize words: The study participants were shown pictures of objects which were named either “correctly” or “incorrectly.” Simultaneously, the scientist analyzed the brain activities of the participants using electroencephalography (EEG). If the researchers detected an effect in the EEG known as N400, they knew that the child registered the incorrect word. This means they had established a stable connection between objects and their names; they had learned the word.

“Children with cochlear implants could help us understand the general processes of language acquisition and determine which single steps are age-dependent,” said Angela D. Friederici, study leader and head of MPI CBS. “We now know that age does not affect how fast children learn words. On the contrary, they seem to catch up even if they were previously disadvantaged.”

Upcoming studies should now focus on why some of the affected children, despite these findings, struggle to reach the level of their contemporaries with normal hearing.

Source: The Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences

Image: The Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences